11/08/04
Are You A Deviant?
(Note: Many tests used by psychologists are little more than
mumbo-jumbo masquerading as science. In this particular case concerning a
purported test of sexual deviancy , I attempt to show you why such tests
are often invalid, and why, in order to protect yourself, you would be
better off by not telling the truth to the psychologist.)
Abel Assessment A
test of sexual interest and deviancy
Well: I can categorically assure you that tests such as these are often little more
than money-makers - but people - men - are very often assessed for
deviancy on the basis of such tests. And so, for example, men in contested
divorce cases might be denied access to their very own children on the grounds
that such tests suggest that they are 'deviant'.
But there is a real problem with this.
these tests ... have virtually no validity when
assessing individuals.
These tests might have some validity in the sense
that they can differentiate statistically between groups of
people, but they have virtually no validity when assessing individuals.
For example, imagine a test that tries to assess
people's weights simply by asking
subjects two questions.
1. What is your gender?
2. What is your height?
On the basis of the answers to these two
questions, such a test would be pretty good at assessing the average
weights of the groups of subjects who fell into the different
categories - men/women - tall/short.
This is because, for example, taller men tend to
be heavier than shorter men, and because men tend to be heavier than women
for any given height.
And so on.
And so, for example, if 100 men took this test
and each said, "I am a man who is x foot tall," then the assessor would, on
average, be able to assess quite well the average weight
of the group.
Of. The. GROUP!
Thus, for example, the assessor would be able to
assess reasonably well the average weight of a particular group of 'tall' 'men'.
But such a test would not tell us much that was
reliable
about the individuals themselves.
And so, for example, on the basis of such a
test, a tall thin man would be much lighter in weight than the
test would assess him to be. He might even be lighter than a short woman.
And the reason for this large error would be that
such a simple test would have failed to take into account the fatness or
thinness of the subjects taking the test. In other words, the test would not have taken
into account all the variables that are relevant when it comes to assessing
weight.
Thus, without knowing about the fatness or the thinness of a particular
man, our test is quite likely to be way off the mark when it comes to
predicting his weight.
Well. When it comes to 'psychological'
matters, the variables that are likely to be relevant to them are not
only positively vast in number and infinitely complex, but they are also very likely to be unknown.
And so while psychological tests might be of
value when it comes to differentiating between groups of people,
they cannot necessarily be relied upon to assess individuals accurately.
For example, one of the most thoroughly
researched areas when it comes to psychological testing is that connected with
the assessment of intelligence.
Some of these tests have been researched
fairly extensively for almost 100 years. And some of them are very good
indeed.
And in some 90+% of cases, these 'good' tests
seem to predict pretty accurately how well individuals are likely to perform in
cognitive tasks that require the use of 'intelligence'.
But there are many occasions
where, for some reason or other, even these thoroughly-researched tests give
results that are way off the mark for particular individuals.
none of these tests should ever
be used to justify serious consequences for any of the individuals
who took them
As such, none of these tests should ever
be used to justify serious consequences for any of the individuals
who took them, because, as yet, they are nowhere near being valid enough nor
accurate enough to warrant such consequences.
And, quite frankly, a quick glance at the Abel
test website should tell you all that you need to know about the motive and
the rationale behind the construction of this particular test.
It's called 'money'.
Look at the boasting (found on this page)
...
Half hour per client
Easy to interpret – one page graph
Probability Values
Computer scored questionnaire summary
The whole thing is almost obscene.
The whole thing is almost obscene.
And in much the same way that Tarot card
readers will wax lyrical about how accurate and valid are their Tarot card
readings in order to persuade people to partake of their services, many
'psychologists' will do the same for the very same reason.
Unsurprisingly.
But there is a further important point to be
made about this so-called test for sexual predilections.
And it is this.
Intelligence tests are also researched thoroughly with regard to how they
predict future behaviour.
In other words, the researchers will check to see whether or not the people whom they
have tested (e.g. children in a school) did, in fact, end up many years later
performing as expected.
If not, they will jigger the various weightings and the factors so that future
intelligence testing is likely to be more accurate.
And, as the years go by, more accurate will they indeed become - up to a
point.
But the Abel test has not been developed in this way. It has
not had time to develop in this way. And, further, this test
appears to be based simply
on comparisons between alleged molesters and alleged non-molesters who answered
various questions and looked at sexy pictures
there is a fundamental flaw in the thinking behind this
test ...
As such, there is a fundamental flaw in the thinking behind this test that
could easily lead those who might be investigated by it to incriminate
themselves when all they are doing is trying to demonstrate their innocence by
being honest.
Basically the Abel Test tries to determine whether or not an individual is likely
to engage in 'deviant' behaviour. And so what the constructors of this test have basically done is this.
They have asked those convicted of deviant behaviour to complete the test and
they have compared the answers of these individuals to those who have not been
convicted of any such offence. They have then determined which questions discriminate best between
the two groups.
And so, for example, if those who have been convicted of deviant behaviour
mostly answer Yes to questions 1, 2 and 3, whereas those who have never been
convicted of such a thing mostly answer No, then the thinking is that if
psychologists test someone about whom they know nothing, then they will be able to
determine (with some degree of probability) which group they fall into.
And so if an individual is asked to take the test and he answers Yes to
questions 1, 2 and 3, then he will be categorised in the same group as the
convicted offenders (i.e. it will be argued that he is likely to engage in
deviant behaviours) whereas if he answers No, then he will be categorised as
someone who is not likely to offend.
But there is a truly fundamental problem here. And it is this.
Those who have already been convicted of deviant behaviour have no real
incentive for lying, or for deceiving the psychologists. Indeed, they might even
be getting help from the psychologists and are, therefore, only too willing to help
them construct their tests.
those who have not been convicted
of anything might well have good reasons for lying
On the other hand, those who have not been convicted of
anything might well have good reasons for lying and for trying to deceive the
testers.
For example, imagine that questions 1, 2 and 3 are as follows.
1. Do you have fantasies of having sex with young girls?
2. Do you enjoy fantasies of rape?
3. Do you dream about orgies?
Well.
Convicted offenders might happily answer Yes to such questions.
After all, they have already been caught!
But how would 'normal' men who have not been convicted of any
such things answer such questions?
Might they not lie?
Might they not say to themselves, "No way am I telling the truth about
my fantasies to these people. They'll think I'm a pervert!"
And so the point is this.
The constructors of the test will have been misled into believing that their
test can be used to discriminate between those who are likely to engage
in deviant behaviours and those who are not.
But the truth of the matter might be that their test merely discriminates
between those who have been caught engaging in such behaviours
and, perhaps, ordinary men who have not engaged in such things (or
who have not been caught engaging in such things) - but who are not
going to tell the truth about their fantasies in the tests - for obvious
reasons.
In other words, the test might have been constructed by comparing men who
have no reason to lie (because they have already been caught) with men who have
every reason to lie (because they do not wish to be seen as deviant).
As such, if you are tested, and you happen to have some of the
fantasies above, and, for the sake of honesty, you admit to them, then you
will be categorised as someone who is likely to engage in deviant
behaviours whereas, in fact, you might be no different from ordinary innocent
men! - who, for the most part, do not engage in such behaviours but who, in
fact, might
fantasise about them without being prepared to admit to this.
the test is not actually discriminating between
'deviants' and 'normals',
In other words, the test is not actually discriminating between
'deviants' and 'normals', it is discriminating between, ...
1. Those who have been caught engaging in deviant behaviours and those
who have not been caught doing so.
and/or
2. Those who do not feel the need to lie and those who do feel the need
to lie.
Well. OK. The three questions above are a gross simplification of what the
Abel test involves. But the criticisms of the test - as outlined above - give some indication of
just how flawed this test is likely to be.
Imagine, for example, that you wanted to construct a
test to figure out which men were likely to commit
adultery.
Imagine, for example, that you wanted to construct a test to figure out which men
were likely to commit adultery. And, just for the sake of
argument, assume that all men have fantasies about committing
adultery.
You take a group of convicted adulterers and, in the presence
of their wives, you ask them this question.
Have you ever had thoughts about committing adultery?
They all answer Yes.
Unsurprisingly.
You now take a group of men who have never been convicted of committing
adultery and you ask them, in the presence of their wives, the same question.
Have you ever had thoughts about committing adultery?
They all answer No.
Unsurprisingly.
A perfect correlation!
So. Do you now have the basis for a valid test that determines
the likelihood of a man committing adultery?
Hmm.
Let's see.
A wife brings her never-convicted husband along and says to you, "Please
give him the test."
And you duly oblige.
You ask him, "Have you ever had thoughts about committing
adultery?"
If he says Yes, does this mean that he is extremely likely to
commit adultery? - after all, your test validation procedures showed a perfect
correlation - and from this it follows that he will certainly
commit adultery.
But this is clearly nonsense. He might just be a rare soul who is being
honest about his thoughts.
And what if he says, "No, I have never had any thoughts about committing
adultery."
Well, according to your test, this man is definitely not going to commit
adultery.
But do you necessarily believe him? - especially as his wife squints her eyes
with menace and scrutinises his face closely.
The reality, of course, is that whichever way he answers, you
will most likely have learned precious little about the likelihood of him
committing adultery, even though when you constructed your test it showed a perfect
correlation between the answers to the test and the (apparent) behaviours of the
subjects who gave them.
And the main reason that a test such as this would be invalid
is because it has been developed by means of comparing the responses of a category
of people who have a large incentive to lie (viz, the group of
people who have not been convicted of anything but who,
nevertheless, would not likely wish to reveal their private thoughts) to those
who have already been caught and who have far less of an incentive to lie.
if you find yourself being assessed
by the Abel test then giving honest answers is probably not a wise thing to
do!
And, regretfully, as such, if you find yourself being assessed
by the Abel test then giving honest answers is probably not a wise thing to
do!
In other words, it is probably best to respond to the test in much the same
way as did those supposedly non-deviant men likely respond to it; viz,
by lying - if they are, in fact, 'deviant' - or by not lying - if, in fact, they
are non-deviant.
Either way, these men said, "No Siree. Not me. I never have such
thoughts."
And the upshot is that if you are asked to take the test then,
whether or not you have deviant fantasies, it is best to give the impression
that you don't - because honesty could easily land you in serious trouble even
though you have not, and will not, ever engage in deviant behaviour.
Part of the test appears to involve subjects looking at
various sexy pictures
Added Note 1:
Part of the test appears to involve subjects looking at various
sexy pictures - some of them allegedly 'deviant' and some of them not - and assessing how
much the subjects like them by observing how long they choose to look at the
various categories of pictures.
Well. Apart from the fact that it must be blindingly obvious to subjects what
the whole thing is about - which means that they can easily 'lie' by, for
example, looking longer at the non-deviant pictures - there remains no real
justification for assuming that the longer that one looks at a picture, the more
is one being sexually aroused by it. For example, it might simply be the case
that a subject has so little experience of viewing material contained in
'deviant' pictures that he is far more interested in looking at such pictures
than at the ones which contain more 'normal' material and, hence, material which
he has probably seen many times before.
In other words, his 'interest' in 'deviant' pictures as reflected solely by
how long he looks at them might be nothing more than a reflection of his added
curiosity over something that he has rarely encountered before.
Human brains are much more complex than psychologists often assume them to
be.
Added Note 2:
The type of video games that I have most enjoyed in the past
are those wherein various warriors of old beat the hell out of each other. They impale
each other with axes and swords. They beat each other ceaselessly with fists and
feet. They knock each other unconscious. And, allegedly, they are 100% to do
with 'violence'.
But, in fact, they are all fantasy, totally unrealistic, and
no-one actually gets hurt.
Indeed, human beings are perfectly capable of enjoying fictional
representations of situations that they would positively hate to encounter in
real life.
But the real point here is this.
If the public was ever indoctrinated successfully with the view
that playing 'violent' video games was a valid mark of serious 'deviancy', then
I would not be admitting to my enjoyment of 'violent' video games to psychologists who were
assessing me for such deviancy.
And nor would most men who enjoyed such games.
Unless, of course, they had already been caught playing such games.
...
Also see the Ink Blot Test - which is a
completely bogus test that is used by many clinical psychologists to assess, for
example, the suitability of a man to be a father to his own children.
To gain some further insight into just how discreditable the results of
testing for psychological 'deviancy' might be, the following article describes
how a test that simply tests for drug abuse with a purported 99% accuracy level
will, in fact, lead to about one-third of those who fail the test
being inaccurately labelled as drug abusers. (A basic
understanding of probability theory is required.)
We like to think these tests are at least 99% accurate, and yet, horror stories of spurious results seem to abound. Take company-wide drug testing, opponents may claim that at least a third of those identified as drug users will actually be innocent. If we assume the test is 99% accurate, this claim sounds ridiculous. But is
it?
|